FUSS IFPS II Revised_clean_Supporting Statement Part B

FUSS IFPS II Revised_clean_Supporting Statement Part B.doc

Followup Study for Infant Feeding Practices Study II

OMB: 0910-0696

Document [doc]
Download: doc | pdf


Followup Study for Infant Feeding Practices Study II


0910-NEW


SUPPORTING STATEMENT



B. Statistical Methods

1. Respondent Universe and Sampling Methods


The respondent universe is all women who participated in the Infant Feeding Practices Study II and completed at least two questionnaires after their infants were born. The total number of women who qualify is 2,847. It is expected that the contractor will be able to find current contact information for 2,562 of these mothers, and that 2,051 will answer the questionnaire (80%) either by mailed questionnaire or by telephone interview.

Data analysis will be conducted using SAS 9.1 statistical software. A statistical power analysis based on Casagrande, 1978 and Fleiss 1981 via the calculations at the website http://department.obg.cuhk.edu.hk/researchsupport/Sample_size_Comp2Prop.asp

indicates that if we test with 95 percent confidence, we will have 80 percent power to detect differences in two groups of 2.5 to 4 percent with about 900 respondents in each group when the overall prevalence of an outcome is 10 percent or less, which is likely to be the case for a number of health outcomes. We will have 80 percent power to detect a difference of 5 percent between two groups of about 1,000 respondents each when the overall prevalence of the outcome is 22.5 percent.

Specific information used for the power analysis is listed below. Because power depends in part on how many respondents are in each of the groups compared, we have first listed the percentage in various feeding characteristic groups, as found in the IFPS II.

Feeding characteristic

Prevalence

In IFPS II (%)

Number in each dichotomous group in the Follow Up Study based on total sample size of 2051




Fed cow milk at 10-1/2 months

17

349/1702

Fed other allergenic foods at 10-1/2 months

30

615/1436

Exclusively breastfed at 3 months

36

738/1313

Fed infant expressed milk

60

1231/820

Breastfed at least 6 m

50

1025/1026

Fed breast milk as 100% of milk feeds at month 9

27

554/1497

Put to bed with a bottle (10-1/2 m)

20

410/1641

Fed less than 1 daily serving of either fruit or vegetable in late infancy

15

308/1743



The table below shows the minimal detectable difference between select groups for various outcomes. The two feeding characteristics chosen for display are the one with half of the sample in each group (breastfed at least six months) and the one with the greatest difference in the size of the groups (fed less than 1 daily serving of fruit or vegetable in late infancy). The minimal difference for the other feeding characteristics will lie between these two. References for the prevalence estimates are given below the table.

Outcome

Prevalence in children (%)

Minimum detectable difference



Breastfed at least 6 months

Fed <1 daily fruit or vegetable

Food allergy, parent-reported symptoms

35 (a)

6

9

Food allergy, diagnosed

6 (a)

4

4

Asthma

6 (b)

4

4

Atopy

15 (a)

5

6.5

Childhood obesity

19 (c)

5

6.5

Inadequate intake of fruit

74 (e)

5.5

8

Poor psychosocial development (Parent reports at least some difficulties on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

45 (d)

6.5

9


a. Lee, LA and Burks, AW. Food allergies: prevalence, molecular characterization, and treatment/prevention strategies. Annu Rev Nutr. 2006;26:539-565.


b. Ip S, Chung M, Raman G et al. Breastfeeding and maternal and infant health outcomes in developed countries. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep) 2007;1-186.

c. Ogden, CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, McDowell MA, Tabak CJ, Flegal KM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999-2004. JAMA. 2006;295(13):1549-1555.

d. Kramer MS, Fombonne E, Igumnov S et al. Effects of prolonged and exclusive breastfeeding on child behavior and maternal adjustment: evidence from a large, randomized trial. Pediatrics 2008;121:E435-E440.

e. Lorson, BA, Melgar-Quinonez, HR, Taylor, CA. Correlates of fruit and vegetable intakes in US children. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:474-478.



2. Procedures for the Collection of Information

Data for this study will be collected in two ways. Most respondents will complete mailed questionnaires, but those who do not respond will be administered a telephone interview if possible. A number of contacts are planned with the respondents, and different versions of the materials will be needed depending on whether the person is a member of the mail or the internet panel because the incentive structure is a little different for the mail and internet panel members. The incentive description, and therefore material that mentions the incentive, will also be different if the respondent is a former panel member (i.e., currently a non-panel member). The contact points and materials for each subsample are listed in Appendix A.

All eligible participants from the Infant Feeding Practices Study II (IFPS II) will be sent a personalized pre-notification letter. If the participant is still a panel member, the letter will be signed by the panel spokeswoman alias, Marie Brighton. If the person is not a current panel member, the letter will be signed by the Vice President of Synovate. This letter will serve two purposes. First, it will inform the respondents about the nature and purpose of a questionnaire that will be sent to them shortly thereafter and will encourage their cooperation. Second, returned letters will give the contractor a list of people who have changed addresses since the last data collection of the IFPS II so that their current contact information can be traced. The contractor will use commercial tracing organizations to find updated contact information. The expected successful contact rate is 90% of the total eligible sample. The pre-notification letters are shown in Appendix B.

An introductory letter (Appendix C) will be mailed to the sample with the questionnaire (see Appendix D for the pilot version of the questionnaire) and a short demographic survey (Appendix E) for women who are no longer panel members. Two weeks later, post card reminders will be mailed (Appendix F). One month after the initial questionnaire mailing, a second complete package will be sent to non-respondents, along with a revised introductory letter (Appendix G).

The completed mail questionnaires will be sent by respondents directly to the contractor, who will scan them and import them into Fast and Accurate Questionnaire Scanning System (FAQSS) to capture the data. FAQSS is used by the Census Bureau and is regarded as the best data capture system available. It can read marks in check boxes and numeric responses. Use of this system eliminates error from human data entry. Verbatim comments by respondents will be hand-entered by scanning operators and then coded in collaboration with FDA’s Project Officer.

Two weeks after the second mailing, non-respondents will be contacted for a telephone interview to collect the same information that was requested in the mail questionnaire, including the demographic data if necessary. (See Appendix H for the introduction to the telephone interview.) The telephone interview will be based on the mailed questionnaire, which will be used to program a computer assisted telephone interview. Specially trained professional interviewers will conduct the telephone interviews. Quality control will be assured by periodic monitoring of on-going interviews throughout the study. The telephone interview data will be entered into a data base during the interview.

Data analysis will be conducted using SAS 9.1 statistical software. A statistical power analysis based on Casagrande, 1978 and Fleiss 1981 via the calculations at the website http://department.obg.cuhk.edu.hk/researchsupport/Sample_size_Comp2Prop.asp

indicates that if we test with 95 percent confidence, we will have 80 percent power to detect differences in two groups of 2.5 to 4 percent with about 900 respondents in each group when the overall prevalence of an outcome is 10 percent or less, which is likely to be the case for a number of health outcomes. We will have 80 percent power to detect a difference of 5 percent between two groups of about 1,000 respondents each when the overall prevalence of the outcome is 22.5 percent.

  3. Methods to Maximize Response Rates and Deal with Non-response

Because this is a follow up study with a sample based on respondents to a previous study, each respondent is critically important to the success of the data collection. As noted in B2, all sample members will be mailed a pre-notification letter as the first contact for this study. The total sample will be mailed a post card reminder two weeks after the initial mailing of the survey packet. Those who are non-respondents will be sent a second complete package one month after the initial questionnaire mailing, and two weeks after the second mailing, the remaining non-respondents will be contacted for a telephone interview. The number of responses is expected to be 2,051, which is a response rate of 80 percent based on the 2,562 mothers on whom we expect to obtain current contact information.

Our estimate that we will be able to find contact information for 90% of the respondents to the IFPS II is based on several factors. First, the IFPS II sample was relatively stable in their residence, given that they were on a consumer opinion panel and that a very large percentage of them answered numerous questionnaires by mail when their child was an infant. It is likely that most will continue to be residentially stable and therefore easier to trace than some elements of the US population. Second, the panel administration has contact information for all of them that was current about five years ago because the IFPS II continued through the child’s first birthday. In some instances, the panel will also have names of other family members.


A description of the tracing procedures follows. Drawing upon Experian/MSG’s in-house Business and Household databases, its GENESYS system provides sample/list enhancement services specifically focused on the needs of researchers.  Computerized forward and reverse matching appends phone numbers, addresses, and demographic information to lists.  We plan to use Experian/MSG’s Enhanced Match, which, on average yields 10-15% more matches.  To conduct the Enhanced Matching, MSG uses 4 databases to append addresses and phone numbers.  The first database is the TARGUS database.  TARGUS has hundreds of data sources and a unique membership in the U.S. Telecommunications Network.  The database is updated several times daily.  Any records that do not return a match are run against the 3 consumer databases MSG has in-house including: InfoUSA, Experian, and Acxiom to locate a match.  These databases are compiled from various sources including white page listings, public information, and other proprietary sources.   


As an additional step, Synovate will verify the mailing address for each sample member by performing a look-up of name and address records with the National Change of Address (NCOA) files.  ZIP codes will be updated on existing addresses, and street name designations will be brought into compliance with Postal Service requirements, increasing the deliverability and thereby increasing the contact rate.  We will also obtain and append telephone numbers for sample members, where available.


We have several reasons for assuming that this study will have a response rate of 80 percent. The first reason is that these same people had high response rates in the infant part of the sample, which occurred during a time that most mothers find challenging. The response rates were 82.9% for the birth screener, 76.9% for the neonatal (first month) questionnaire, and 83.1% for the month 2 questionnaire. In the IFPS II, 76.6 percent of respondents answered at least seven of the total twelve questionnaires. We believe that many respondents will be more inclined to respond because they were part of the earlier study, i.e., because they have already committed time to the study. They all have experience answering the type of questionnaire we will send and doing so by mail. In addition, the current response rate for telephone surveys in the panel is relatively high and will be able to obtain data from many of those who do not answer by mail.


This vendor’s current average panel response rate for mail studies is about 40% without an incentive and its response rate for telephone studies is about 80%. The contractor estimated a higher than average mail response rate (60%) because we are offering an incentive, because respondents should find this survey more interesting than average, and because of the past experience respondents have with this study.  They estimated a lower than average telephone response rate [(513/1024) = 50%] because the questionnaire is longer than most they administer and because they will be calling respondents who were unable or unwilling to answer by mail.


The incentive offered for this study, either $10 or $15, is about average for a 20 minute survey. The incentive usually offered depends on the survey topic and the expected respondent interest in the subject. For this study, interest is expected to be high and therefore the incentive will be adequate even for the respondents that take a little longer to complete it.


4. Test of Procedures or Methods to be Undertaken

The questionnaire is based on previously used questions to the extent possible, modified as necessary for written administration. This decision will enable us to compare some estimates with nationally representative data and will reduce the amount of time needed for developing questions and pre-testing. In all cases, we have determined that the questions are in the public domain or we have explicitly obtained permission to use them. Table 3 shows the source of the questions for each topic.

Table 3. Source of questions for Follow Up Study, IFPS II

Topic

Source of questions or style of questions

Outcomes


Psychosocial development

National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH); National Health Interview Survey (NHIS); Strengths and Limitations Questionnaire (used in the PROBIT study (Kramer et al., 2007)) and in the Millennium Cohort Study (Sacker et al., 2006) ; new questions

Child overweight or obesity

National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES); new questions

Dental caries

NSCH; new questions

Child health, asthma

NSCH; custom analysis of 20 most frequent reasons children 6 to 7 years old visited physician offices in 2008 (National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey); NHIS; NHANES

Physical development

NSCH

Child food allergy

Infant Feeding Practices Study II (IFPS II); Food Safety Survey; new questions

Food group consumption

NHANES diet screener; IFPS II; Harvard Children’s Nutrition Questionnaire, new questions;

Maternal overweight or obesity

IFPS II



Confounders


Child care arrangements

NSCH

School arrangements

New questions

Cognitive stimulation at home

HOME questionnaire (Bradley et al., 2001)    

Dental hygiene

New questions

Physical activity of child and mother

NSCH; new questions

Screen time

NSCH; new questions

Sleep patterns

New questions

Eating environment

Youth Physical Activity and Nutrition Survey (YPANS)

Child Eating Behavior

Child eating behavior questionnaire (Wardle et al., 2001)

Maternal control of child’s eating

Child Feeding Questionnaire (Birch, et al., 2001)

Presence of pets

New question

Exposure to inhaled contaminants other than cigarette smoke

New questions

Family medical history

IFPS II; new questions

Maternal depression

NHANES (10 item) (Radloff 1977)

Exposure to cigarettes

IFPS II

Pregnancies subsequent to sample child

National Maternal and Infant Health Survey follow up

Maternal employment

IFPS II; new questions

Health insurance

New question

Participation in government nutrition programs

NSCH

During the questionnaire development phase, cognitive interviews were conducted with six respondents to test the new and modified questions. Although many of the questions are from previous data collection instruments, the total questionnaire will present the questions in a different context from the original, and for many questions, the form of collection will be changed from an in-person interview to a written questionnaire or a telephone interview. Therefore, a pilot study of 91 mail and 9 telephone respondents will be conducted to test the questionnaire after OMB approves the data collection. It is expected that the pilot test will refine some of the questions but not result in substantial changes in the questionnaire. The materials for the pilot study are included in the Appendices showing each specific material for the main study, except that the debriefing questionnaire is shown in Appendix I.

The method for the pilot test will be similar to that of the main study with a few exceptions because of the different sample and different purpose. The contractor will select the sample to match the IFPS II sample on the demographic characteristics of race, income, education, region, household size, and age plus 6 years (because the IFPS II sample will be contacted six years after the prenatal questionnaire, on which we asked age). The contractor will identify 250 female panel members that match the above demographic characteristics and that have a 6-year-old child (because the IFPS II sample will be contacted when the target child is 6 years old). A low response rate has been assumed for the pilot study because it is urgent to obtain the target sample size of 100 completed questionnaires in a short period of time.

A prenotification letter (Appendix B) will be sent to the selected sample. Half of the sample will be offered an incentive of $10 value in points and half will be offered $15 in points value so that we can test whether a higher incentive yields a higher response rate. The panel administrators recommended the two different points values based on the length of the survey. A few weeks after the prenotification letter, the sample will be sent an introduction letter (Appendix C) along with the pilot questionnaire (Appendix D) and the debriefing questionnaire (Appendix I). Note that the estimated burden for the pilot study is greater than for the main study because we are asking them to complete the debriefing questionnaire in addition to the study questionnaire. Two weeks after the questionnaire mailing, a post card reminder (Appendix F) will be sent to all sample members. A week later, nine sample members who have not responded will be contacted for a telephone interview to collect the same information that was requested in the mailed questionnaire, using the telephone introduction shown in Appendix H. These mailings and the telephone interview timing are like those proposed for the main study except that in the main study, we will have in addition a second mailing of the questionnaire.

The unedited data from the pilot study will be made into a SAS data base and analyzed for amount of item non-response, inconsistent responses between and within questions, incorrect skips, and multiple responses given to questions that request only one response. The responses to the debriefing questionnaires will be read and coded manually.

We will obtain the following information from analyzing the answers to the pilot test:

  • whether the skip instructions are clear (by analyzing those who should have skipped compared with those who did skip each question)

  • whether any questions generate a large amount of non-response (by reviewing the number of missing responses for each question)

  • whether different questions that ask similar information result in inconsistent responses (by checking the responses of such questions against each other)

  • whether questions with multiple responses have inconsistent responses or have a written response in the “other” category that is the same as one of the listed responses

  • whether any response options are given by either no one or everyone

  • whether items that we believe should have only one answer receive multiple responses.


If we find that any skip instructions are not clear, we will consider using larger or bolder font for the instructions, rearranging questions to minimize the number of questions to be skipped or the location of the place to begin after a skip, or work with the contractor to identify other possible remedies.


If any questions receive a large amount of non-response, we will examine the characteristics of the non-responders to determine whether the question might not apply to their situation; we will examine the placement of the item (for example, was it the beginning of continuing after a skip, or did it have other such placement characteristics that might cause nonresponse?); we will examine cognitive interview responses to the question to determine whether they indicate a lack of clarity that we had not addressed. The remedy we decide on will depend on what we determine to be the cause of the non-response.


If similar questions generate inconsistent responses, we will evaluate whether respondents might misinterpret one of the questions by reviewing the context of the two questions and by examining the characteristics of the respondents with inconsistent responses; we will examine cognitive interview responses to the question to determine whether they indicate a lack of clarity or possible multiple interpretations that we had not addressed. The remedy may be a rewording of one of the items or a rearrangement of one of the questions. We will use similar procedures if responses within a multiple response question are inconsistent (for example, the respondent chooses an option and also chooses “none of these”).


If some response options are chosen by no one, we will evaluate whether the option is likely to be needed in a larger sample. If experts agree that it probably will have a very low or zero frequency in the main sample, we will delete the response option. Because more white space is better in a written questionnaire, there is a cost to keeping options that are not needed. However, if the item measures a low incident condition of importance, we will keep it. If some questions are answered the same by everyone, we will evaluate the need for the question in the main study. If the characteristic is obvious and not likely to vary, we will delete the question.


If some items that we thought should have only one answer receive multiple answers, we will evaluate the combinations of responses chosen and the characteristics of the respondents who give the response to determine whether the question needs to be revised to a “mark all that apply” type of response or if it should be clarified or be given additional response options.


We will obtain the following information from the debriefing questionnaire sent with the pilot questionnaire. Most of this information was obtained from the respondents to the cognitive interviews, but we want to know for a larger number of mothers:

  • Range of length of time it takes respondents to answer the total questionnaire (self-reported in response to a specific question on the debriefing questionnaire)

  • whether most mothers in this sample have a scale for measuring a person’s weight at home and what they did about answering the child’s weight question if they did not have a scale

  • what percent of mothers in the pilot sample had difficulty with the instructions for measuring height

  • what percent of mothers found it hard to remember over the 12 month reference period

  • what kinds of situations mothers have that cause any questions to be hard to answer.


If we find that the questionnaire takes longer than the 20 minutes on average that we expect, we will consider the following possible actions: Increase the response burden estimates we have given in the supporting statement; increase the incentive; delete some questions to shorten the questionnaire. If we believe that we must shorten the questionnaire, we will consider deleting items with these characteristics: request details about some low prevalence conditions, have no variation in the pilot data; ask for lower priority information.


If we find that some mothers do not have a scale and that those who did not own a scale also did not have access to a scale (for example, at a friend’s or relative’s home), we will first evaluate whether the level of missing data that will be caused by this lack is acceptable. If the number missing a scale is too large, we will consider the possibility of adding money to the contract to fund obtaining permission from the mothers and then contacting their health care providers about the child’s weight. This option will require additional review by FDA’s Research Involving Human Subjects Committee. Because all six of the respondents to the cognitive interviews had scales, we expect that nearly all mothers will have a scale.


If mothers have difficulty with the instructions for measuring height, we will simplify the instructions and then test the new instructions with several mothers whom we will obtain from the panel in the same manner as the cognitive interviews. Because none of the six respondents to the cognitive interviews had difficulty with the instructions, we believe that nearly all mothers will be able to follow them easily.


If mothers indicate that that they had difficulty remembering over the 12 month reference period or write in other problems with some questions, we will evaluate the respondent characteristics, the reason for the difficulty, if given, and the details of the mother’s special situation, if given. We will determine how likely it is that the difficulty applies to many other mothers and whether we can clarify the question without changing it substantially before changing the item. The mothers in the cognitive interviews were able to recall information over the reference period.


We will compare response rates between the two groups that receive the different incentive amounts to see if the larger incentive leads to a higher response rate for this type of respondent (by comparing response rates of the groups that receive the $10 and the $15 incentive). With a sample size of 50 in each group, and assuming an overall 80 percent response rate, we will be able to detect a significant difference of 23% or greater at p<.05 and 80 percent power. If the two incentive levels yield a similar response rate, we will use the lower amount for the main study.

The telephone interview part of the pilot test will show us how long the questionnaire takes when administered orally, give an indication of whether respondents react favorably to being called about a questionnaire they received in the mail but did not complete in writing, and show whether any of the questions need to be re-worded in order to obtain the information orally. For example, some of the multiple response questions may be better understood if each response is answered. We will ask the telephone interviewers to give their general impressions of how well each interview went and whether they recognized any particular problems. However, their training is for consistent administration of a questionnaire, not for probe questions as one would do in a cognitive interview, and therefore, we will not ask them to follow up with any questions in addition to those in the questionnaire. An additional reason not to add questions to the telephone interview is that we want to test the length of the oral administration of the questionnaire. Because we are looking for areas between the two types of data collection that may have large differences, we believe that we can obtain the information we need from a small number of telephone respondents.

  1. Individuals Consulted on Statistical Aspects and Individuals Collecting and/or Analyzing Data

The data will be collected by Synovate under a task order, and social scientists from FDA and Centers for Control and Prevention will conduct the data analysis. The names and contact information for the individuals who have consulted on statistical aspects of the study are listed in the table below.

Name

Title

Organization

Telephone number

Email

Valerie DiPaula, Ph.D.

Project director

Synovate

703-663-7243

[email protected]

Sara B. Fein, Ph.D.

Consumer Science Specialist

FDA/CFSAN

301-436-1824

[email protected]

Cary Chen, MPH

Staff Fellow

FDA/CFSAN

301-436-1844

[email protected]

Ann DiGirolamo, Ph.D., MPH

Senior Technical Advisor, Early Childhood Development and HIV/AIDS

CARE USA

404-979-9119

[email protected]

Rouwei Li, M.D., Ph.D.

Epidemiologist

CDC/Division of Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Obesity

770-488-8126

[email protected]

Laurence Grummer-Strawn, Ph.D.

Branch chief

CDC/Division of Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Obesity

770-488-5702

[email protected]



Reference List


(1) Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 1999;70:525-535.

(2) Angelsen NK, Vik T, Jacobsen G, Bakketeig LS. Breast feeding and cognitive development at age 1 and 5 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood 2001;85:183-188.

(3) Baker JL, Gamborg M, Heitmann BL, Lissner L, Sorensen TIA, Rasmussen KM. Breastfeeding reduces postpartum weight retention. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2008;88:1543-1551.

(4) Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA. Early Flavor Learning and Its Impact on Later Feeding Behavior. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition 2009;48:S25-S30.

(5) Birch EE, Garfield S, Hoffman DR, Uauy R, Birch DG. A randomized controlled trial of early dietary supply of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and mental development in term infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2000;42:174-181.

(6) Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite 2001;36:201-210.

(7) Bradley RH, Caldwell BM, Rock SL, Hamrick HM, Harris P. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment - Development of A Home Inventory for Use with Families Having Children 6 to 10 Years Old. Contemporary Educational Psychology 1988;13:58-71.

(8) Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, Mcadoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part I: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development 2001;72:1844-1867.

(9) Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, Burchinal M, Mcadoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development 2001;72:1868-1886.

(10) Buchmann AF, Kopf D, Westphal S et al. Impact of Early Parental Child-Rearing Behavior on Young Adults' Cardiometabolic Risk Profile: A Prospective Study. Psychosomatic Medicine 2010;72:156-162.

(11) Carnell S, Wardle J. Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: Validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite 2007;48:104-113.

(12) Casagrande JT, Pike MC, Smith PG. Improved Approximate Formula for Calculating Sample Sizes for Comparing 2 Binomial Distributions. Biometrics 1978;34:483-486.

(13) Chan GM, Borschel MW, Jacobs JR. Effects of Human-Milk Or Formula Feeding on the Growth, Behavior, and Protein Status of Preterm Infants Discharged from the Newborn Intensive-Care Unit. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 1994;60:710-716.

(14) de Jong C, Kikkert HK, Fidler V, Hadders-Algra M. The Groningen LCPUFA study: no effect of postnatal long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in healthy term infants on neurological condition at 9 years. British Journal of Nutrition 2010;104:566-572.

(15) Dodd KW, Guenther PM, Freedman LS et al. Statistical methods for estimating usual intake of nutrients and foods: A review of the theory. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 2006;106:1640-1650.

(16) Faith MS, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Kerns J, Storey M, Stunkard AJ. Parental feeding attitudes and styles and child body mass index: Prospective analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Pediatrics 2004;114:E429-E436.

(17) Farrow CV, Blissett J. Controlling feeding practices: Cause or consequence of early child weight? Pediatrics 2008;121:E164-E169.

(18) Fein SB, Labiner-Wolfe J, Shealy KR, Li RW, Chen J, Grummer-Strawn LM. Infant Feeding Practices Study II: Study methods. Pediatrics 2008;122:S28-S35.

(19) Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Shannon FT. Breastfeeding and Subsequent Social Adjustment in Six to Eight-Year-Old Children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 1987;28:379-386.

(20) Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ. Breast feeding and later psychosocial adjustment. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology 1999;13:144-157.

(21) Fisher JO, Birch LL, Smiciklas-Wright H, Picciano MF. Breast-feeding through the first year predicts maternal control in feeding and subsequent toddler energy intakes. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 2000;100:641-646.

(22) Fleiss JL. Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley, 1981.

(23) Giwercman C, Halkjaer LB, Jensen SM, Bonnelykke K, Lauritzen L, Bisgaard H. Increased risk of eczema but reduced risk of early wheezy disorder from exclusive breast-feeding in high-risk infants. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 2010;125:866-871.

(24) Golding J, Rogers IS, Emmett PM. Association between breast feeding, child development and behaviour. Early Human Development 1997;49:S175-S184.

(25) Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal Depression and Child Psychopathology: A Meta-Analytic Review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 2011;14:1-27.

(26) Greer FR, Sicherer SH, Burks AW, Comm Nutr & Section Allergy. Effects of early nutritional interventions on the development of atopic disease in infants and children: The role of maternal dietary restriction, breastfeeding, timing of introduction of complementary foods, and hydrolyzed formulas. Pediatrics 2008;121:183-191.

(27) Griffiths LJ, Dowda M, Dezateux C, Pate R. Associations between sport and screen-entertainment with mental health problems in 5-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 2010;7.

(28) Guilbert TW, Stern DA, Morgan WJ, Martinez FD, Wright AL. Effect of breastfeeding on lung function in childhood and modulation by maternal asthma and atopy. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine 2007;176:843-848.

(29) Heikkila K, Sacker A, Kelly Y, Renfrew MJ, Quigley MA. Breast feeding and child behaviour in the Millennium Cohort Study. Archives of Disease in Childhood. In press.

(30) Ip S, Chung M, Raman G et al. Breastfeeding and maternal and infant health outcomes in developed countries. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep) 2007;1-186.

(31) Katz Y, Rajuan N, Goldberg MR et al. Early exposure to cow's milk protein is protective against IgE-mediated cow's milk protein allergy. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 2010;126:77-82.

(32) Knitzer J, Theberge S, Johnson K. Reducing Maternal Depression and Its Impact on Young Children - Toward a Responsive Early Childhood Policy Framework. 2, 1-28. 2008. National Center for Children in Poverty - Columbia University Mailman School of Public Health.
Ref Type: Report

(33) Kramer MS, Matush L, Vanilovich I et al. Effect of prolonged and exclusive breast feeding on risk of allergy and asthma: cluster randomised trial. British Medical Journal 2007;335:815-B818.

(34) Kramer MS, Fombonne E, Igumnov S et al. Effects of prolonged and exclusive breastfeeding on child behavior and maternal adjustment: evidence from a large, randomized trial. Pediatrics 2008;121:E435-E440.

(35) Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development - New evidence from a large randomized trial. Archives of General Psychiatry 2008;65:578-584.

(36) Kramer MS, Matush L, Bogdanovich N et al. Health and development outcomes in 6.5-y-old children breastfed exclusively for 3 or 6 mo. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2009;90:1070-1074.

(37) Li RW, Fein SB, Grummer-Strawn LM. Do Infants Fed From Bottles Lack Self-regulation of Milk Intake Compared With Directly Breastfed Infants? Pediatrics 2010;125:E1386-E1393.

(38) Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire: Validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 2007;32:960-972.

(39) Oddy WH, Kendall GE, Li JH et al. The Long-Term Effects of Breastfeeding on Child and Adolescent Mental Health: A Pregnancy Cohort Study Followed for 14 Years. Journal of Pediatrics 2010;156:568-574.

(40) Ogbuanu IU, Karmaus W, Arshad SH, Kurukulaaratchy RJ, Ewart S. Effect of breastfeeding duration on lung function at age 10 years: a prospective birth cohort study. Thorax 2009;64:62-66.

(41) Pieters R, Baumgartner H, Bagozzi R. Biased memory for prior decision making: Evidence from a longitudinal field study. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 2006;99:34-48.

(42) Pomerantz WJ, Timm NL, Gittelman MA. Injury Patterns in Obese Versus Nonobese Children Presenting to a Pediatric Emergency Department. Pediatrics 2010;125:681-685.

(43) Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1977;1:385-401.

(44) Riolo S, Nguyen T-A. Risk factors for depression within obese populations: results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES 2004-2005) [abstract]Riolo S, Nguyen T-A. American Psychiatric Association Annual Meeting 2009;Abstract NR3-026

(45) Sacker A, Quigley MA, Kelly YJ. Breastfeeding and developmental delay: Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. Pediatrics 2006;118:E682-E689.

(46) Simmer K. An intervention promoting exclusive and prolonged breast feeding improved verbal intelligence scores in children at 6.5 years. Evid Based Med 2008;13:181.

(47) Singer MR, Moore LL, Garrahie EJ, Ellison RC. The tracking of nutrient intake in young children: The Framingham Children's Study. American Journal of Public Health 1995;85:1673-1677.

(48) Singhal A, Kennedy K, Lanigan J et al. Nutrition in infancy and long-term risk of obesity evidence from 2 randomized controlled trials. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2010;92:1133-1144.

(49) Skinner AC, Steiner MJ, Henderson FW, Perrin EM. Multiple Markers of Inflammation and Weight Status: Cross-sectional Analyses Throughout Childhood. Pediatrics 2010;125:E801-E809.

(50) Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Houck KS et al. Longitudinal study of nutrient and food intakes of infants aged 2 to 24 months. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 1997;97:496-504.

(51) Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 2002;34:310-315.

(52) Stein AD, Shea S, Basch CE, Contento IR, Zybert P. Variability and Tracking of Nutrient Intakes of Preschool-Children Based on Multiple Administrations of the 24-Hour Dietary Recall. American Journal of Epidemiology 1991;134:1427-1437.

(53) Stuebe AM, Schwarz EB. The risks and benefits of infant feeding practices for women and their children. Journal of Perinatology 2010;30:155-162.

(54) Stunkard AJ, Faith MS, Allison KC. Depression and obesity. Biological Psychiatry 2003;54:330-337.

(55) Tooze JA, Midthune D, Dodd KW et al. A new statistical method for estimating the usual intake of episodically consumed foods with application to their distribution. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 2006;106:1575-1587.

(56) Vollrath ME, Tonstad S, Rothbart MK, Hampson SE. Infant temperament is associated with potentially obesogenic diet at 18 months. Int J Pediatr Obes 2010.

(57) Wang YF, Bentley ME, Zhai FY, Popkin BM. Tracking of dietary intake patterns of Chinese from childhood to adolescence over a six-year follow-up period. Journal of Nutrition 2002;132:430-438.

(58) Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaire. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2001;42:963-970.

(59) Whitaker RC, Orzol SM, Kahn RS. Maternal mental health, substance use, and domestic violence in the year after delivery and subsequent behavior problems in children at age 3 years. Archives of General Psychiatry 2006;63:551-560.

(60) Wojcicki JM, Heyman MB. Let's Move - Childhood Obesity Prevention from Pregnancy and Infancy Onward. New England Journal of Medicine 2010;362:1457-1459.

(61) Ystrom E, Niegel S, Vollrath ME. The impact of maternal negative affectivity on dietary patterns of 18-month-old children in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. Maternal and Child Nutrition 2009;5:234-242.

(62) Ziegler AG, Schmid S, Huber D, Hummel M, Bonifacio E. Early infant feeding and risk of developing type 1 diabetes-associated autoantibodies. Jama-Journal of the American Medical Association 2003;290:1721-1728.

17


File Typeapplication/msword
File TitleFollow Up of Infant Feeding Practices Study II
Authorsfein
Last Modified BySharon Harrison
File Modified2011-11-02
File Created2011-11-02

© 2024 OMB.report | Privacy Policy